Educational Research and Reviews

  • Abbreviation: Educ. Res. Rev.
  • Language: English
  • ISSN: 1990-3839
  • DOI: 10.5897/ERR
  • Start Year: 2006
  • Published Articles: 2008

Full Length Research Paper

Examining the relationship between empathy for others and self-compassion in college students

Rachel M. Daltry
  • Rachel M. Daltry
  • Department of Counseling and Psychological Services, West Chester University of Pennsylvania, 241 Lawrence Center, West Chester, PA 19383, United States.
  • Google Scholar
Kristin E. Mehr
  • Kristin E. Mehr
  • Department of Counseling and Psychological Services, West Chester University of Pennsylvania, 241 Lawrence Center, West Chester, PA 19383, United States.
  • Google Scholar
Lindsay Sauers
  • Lindsay Sauers
  • Department of Counseling and Psychological Services, West Chester University of Pennsylvania, 241 Lawrence Center, West Chester, PA 19383, United States.
  • Google Scholar
Janelle Silbert
  • Janelle Silbert
  • Department of Counseling and Psychological Services, West Chester University of Pennsylvania, 241 Lawrence Center, West Chester, PA 19383, United States.
  • Google Scholar


  •  Received: 26 January 2018
  •  Accepted: 24 September 2018
  •  Published: 25 September 2018

 ABSTRACT

This study examined the correlation between self-compassion and empathy for others in college students in order to better inform clinical work and outreach programming in university counseling centers. Preliminary analyses indicated that gender identity was a confounding variable; therefore, the main analyses were run distinctly for male and female identified participants. There was no important association between self-compassion and empathy for female identified participants.  There was an important connection between self-compassion and empathy for others for male identified participants, indicating that higher self-compassion was linked to lower empathy for others.  Possible explanations along with recommendations are given.

 

Key words: Empathy for others, self-compassion, college students.


 INTRODUCTION

The study of empathy continues to develop, in part due to the increasing field of social neuroscience and the technological advancement that enhance studying neural based empathy (Rueckertet al., 2011). Empathy is the capability to take in other people’s perspective, and to be able to understand and be aware of their feelings, thoughts, and experiences.  Perry et al. (2013) state that empathy is a multi-dimensional construct that has both cognitive and emotional reactions to events experienced by others. Emotional empathy is the inclination to feel what another person feels, while cognitive empathy is the understanding and knowledge of the thoughts and emotions of others without feeling same (Rueckert et al., 2011). 
 
Given that empathy involves the awareness of other peoples’ experiences, it is not surprising that  it  has been connected to the social and reasoning abilities of emotional intelligence, perspective taking, and self-realization (Taylor et al., 2013). These skills are important for developing and maintaining relationships with others, especially for college students who are forging new social networks and connections. Carlo et al. (2012) found that empathy mediates the relationship between connection with peers and prosocial behavior in college students. Thus, it seems that empathy is important for college students and their interpersonal functioning. This connection is important since perceived social support in college students has been linked to experiencing less mental health difficulties and more resilience to cope with stressful situations (Taylor et al., 2014).
 
Compassion and empathy have been linked in the literature,  where  the  constructs  have been shown to be  
 
related to, but also different from, one another.  Brill and Nahmani (2017) talk about an empathic attitude as being the ability to “be with,” while compassion is to “suffer with.”  They focus on the mental element of empathy (that is, how it is an understanding of others’ emotions), while compassion encompasses the whole (emotion, awareness, and action) with the focus on associating with the suffering other (Brill and Nahmani, 2017).  Birnie et al. (2010) further distinguish between empathy and compassion by stating while both involve understanding others’ intellectual and emotional suffering, compassion adds the desire to reduce the suffering noticed. 
 
While compassion for others involves understanding and alleviating their suffering, self-compassion encompasses having concern for one’s own pain and viewing one’s difficulties with understanding, nonjudgment, and knowledge that struggle is common to life (Neff, 2003).  Further, Neff (2003) describes self-compassion as an emotional guideline plan, where negative feelings are seen as common to all humans. In essence, self-compassion is recognizing that mistakes and challenges are part of human beings and that everyone, including oneself, merit compassion (Neff, 2003). 
 
Self-compassion is connected to interpersonal operations in college students.  For instance, self-compassion in college students is positively related to effective interpersonal problem-solving behavior and inversely related to a negative approach to solving problems (Arslan, 2016).  Additionally, in a study in which college students were asked to reflect on a situation of relational conflict, higher self-compassion was linked to higher tendency to give in and lower tendency to
subsume their needs, plus higher relational well-being (Yarnell and Neff, 2013).  Although it seems logical for a positive connection to exist between self-compassion and empathy for others, the limited literature has been varied. For instance, while Birnie et al. (2010) observed that self-compassion was connected to empathy positively, Wei, Wei et al. (2011) did not find a significant relationship. Wei et al. (2011) postulated that their explanation of empathy, not including the elements of common humanity and mindfulness of self-compassion, may have contributed to this lack of relationship. It may also be suggested that those who have high self-compassion could be equally nice to others as they are themselves, while those  with low self-compassion tend to be nicer to others  than to themselves (Neff, 2003). Thus, those with low self-compassion can be empathetic like those with high self-compassion (Wei et al., 2011).
 
Low self-compassion is related to mental suffering in college students, including depression (Neff, 2011), sadness, anxiety, anger, and embarrassment (Leary et al., 2007). Self-compassion is a significant mediator between negative life experiences and suicidal tendency, as indicated  by  depressive  sighs  and suicidal  actions (Chang et al., 2016). As the distress levels of college students continue to rise and university counseling centers struggle to meet these demands, it is important to reexamine and develop resources that better meet the mental health needs of these college students. The aim of the current work is to further examine the association between self-compassion and empathy for others in college students in order to better inform not only clinical work in university counseling centers, but also to improve their outreach programming. 


 METHODS

Participants
 
323 students from a mid-size government university in the northeastern United States participated in this study, from whom data were obtained. Their age on average was 18.95 years (SD = 2.49), ranging from 17 to 47 years. They were identified as female (221; 68.4%), male (96; 29.7%), transgender (1; 0.3%), gender neutral (3; 0.9%), and gender fluid (2; 0.6 %). They identified their ethnicity as: 3 (0.9%) African, 32 (9.9%) African American or Black, 14 (4.3%) Asian American or Pacific Islander, 238 (73.7%) Caucasian/Non-Hispanic, 19 (5.9%) Hispanic or Latino (a), 1 (0.3%) Native American, 2 (0.6%) biracial, 7 (2.2%) multiracial, and 7 (2.2%) “Other.” They identified their sexual orientation as heterosexual (298, 92.3%), gay/lesbian (6, 1.9%), bisexual (11, 3.4%), questioning (5, 1.5%), and “other” (3, 0.9%). Two-hundred and twenty-one (68.4%) classified themselves as 1st year, 62 (19.2%) as 2nd year, 25 (7.7%) as 3rd year, 12 (3.7%) as 4th year, and 3 (0.9%) as 5th year.
 
Measures
 
Demographic questionnaire
 
All the participants were asked to fill in a demographic questionnaire, developed for this study.  The questionnaire contains the participants’ age, gender identity, sexual orientation, year in school, and racial/ethnic background.
 
Self-Compassion Scale (SCS) Neff (2003) is a 26-item self-report consisting of six subscales used for the measurement of global self-compassion dimensions: Mindfulness, Self-Kindness, Over-Identification, Common Humanity, Isolation, and Self-Judgment.  A 5-point Likert scale was used to rate the items: 1 (almost never) to 5 (almost always).  A lot of the items are reverse-scored; scores of the item are totaled to get a global self-compassion score, in which higher scores indicate greater self-compassion. Neff (2003) showed that the SCS  had good convergent and discriminant validity  proven by the great negative relation with  self-criticism (-0.65) measures and great  positive relations with social connectedness measures (0.41) as well as with the Repair (0.55) and Clarity (0.43) subscales of the Trait Meta-Mood Scale, for assessing emotional intelligence. Test–retest reliability for the SCS was 0.93 for more than 3 weeks (Neff, 2003). The SCS internal consistency was 0.92 (Neff, 2003), but was 0.91 in this work.
 
The Basic Empathy Scale (BES); Jolliffe and Farrington (2006) is a 20-item self-report measure with two factors: Cognitive Empathy (9 items) and Affective Empathy (11 items). The Cognitive Empathy subscale is related to understanding the reason an individual has a kind of emotion (for  instance, “I can  explain  my friend’s happiness when she/he does something well”);the Affective Empathy subscale shows  how other people’s emotions are expressed (example., “When close to a friend who is not happy, I always feed unhappy”; Carre et al., 2013).  A 5-point Likert type scale was used to rate the items: 1 (Strongly Disagree) to 5 (Strongly Agree). The sum of the ratings for cognitive empathy items and affective empathy items yielded cognitive and empathy scale scores, respectively. The sum of the two subscales yielded a total empathy score. The BES has convergent validity with measures of perspective taking, alexithymia, and openness (Jolliffe and Farrington, 2006). For reliability, internal consistency estimates range from 0.79 for BES Cognitive empathy to 0.85 for affective empathy (Jolliffe and Farrington, 2006). The BES’s internal consistency  was 0.82 in this work.
 
Procedure
 
This work was accepted by the institutional review board of the university. Solicitation for respondent occurred via psychology courses that made participating in research compulsory. Students selected from different works that they can participate in order to fulfill their requirement. The data were collected and stored online through a secured survey website. Participants consented to get involved after going a consent form online and were then guided with the survey measures. After the survey, a debriefing page informed participants of the hypothesis, method, and logic of the study. They were not given any fund for participating in this study, though students did receive credit toward their research course requirement.


 RESULTS

Preliminary analyses
 
Prior to analyses, one participant was removed due to an outlier score on the Basic Empathy Scale. The skewness and kurtosis values for the Self-Compassion Scale and the Basic Empathy Scale fell within the -2 to +2 acceptable range (Lomax, 2001). To test the demographic variables with likely confusing effect on the main variables, a sequence of multivariate regression analyses was done. The demographic factor was the independent factor, while the main variables were dependent in each analysis. The per comparison alpha level was set to .001 to reduce Type 1 error, while at the same time making an estimate of likely confusing impact. In these analyses, gender identity had an important association with self-compassion (p <0.001) and empathy (p <0.001); therefore gender identity was found to have a confounding influence on the primary variables.
 
Main analyses
 
Given these results, we broadened our overall investigation to include variations in the correlations between self-compassion and empathy based on gender identity, which necessitated removal of the six participants that did not identify as male or female due to the low sample size. A one-way  between  subjects  ANOVA  was calculated to make comparison of the mean scores on the Self-Compassion Scale for males and females.  The mean score for male participants on the SCS was 3.07 (SD = 0.594), while the mean score for females was 2.81 (SD = 0.608). There was an important effect of sex on self-compassion (F (1, 315) = 11.804, p = 0.001).  On the Basic Empathy Scale, the mean score for male participants was 71.57 (SD = 8.78), and the mean score for females was 77.57 (SD = 7.76). With one-way between subjects ANOVA, the means on the BES were compared. Gender has significant impact on empathy (F (1, 315) = 32.818, p <0.001). 
 
A Pearson r was calculated to find the correlation between self-compassion and empathy for both the male and female groups. Based on the sample, there was no great correlation between self-compassion and empathy among female participants. There was a great correlation between self-compassion and empathy among the sample of male participants (r = -0.225, p = 0.028), indicating that as self-compassion increases, empathy decreases. A simple linear regression was then calculated to forecast empathy in relation to self-compassion scores, and there was a great regression equation (F (1, 94) = 5.006, p = 0.028), with an R² of 0.051.


 DISCUSSION

The  current work aims to further examine the correlation between self-compassion and empathy for others in college students in order to better inform not only clinical work in university counseling centers, but also improve their outreach programming. The results showed a great correlation with gender identity and self-compassion and empathy for others, which prompted the analyses for this study to be run separately for male and female identified participants.  Empathy is being sensitive to individual variation, specifically variation related to gender (Schulte-Ruther et al., 2008; Yang et al., 2009; Derntl et al., 2010; Pavlova et al., 2010). Research using self-report measures has consistently and reliably found that women report having more empathy than men (Baron-Cohen and Wheelwright, 2004; Eisenberg and Lennon, 1983; Lam et al., 2012; Rueckert and Naybar, 2008; Stuijfzand et al., 2016). There have been several explanations for this, some highlighting neurological variations between men and women, and the influence of gender roles (Swickert et al., 2016).  Swicker et al. (2016) also suggest that age may be an influencing factor; women had higher levels of empathy than men in young adulthood, but that this gender difference starts to converge in older adults.
 
Female identified participants
 
For  female  identified  participants  there   was   no  great correlation relationship between self-compassion and empathy for others. One possible explanation for this result could be that regardless of one’s level of self-compassion, one can still experience high levels of empathy for others. Female identified participants in this study had higher levels of empathy for others than the male identified participants, supporting past research (Baron-Cohen and Wheelwright, 2004; Eisenberg and Lennon, 1983; Lam et al., 2012) creating even less of a difference between their levels of empathy.  Thus, those with high self-compassion could be equally kind to others as they are to themselves, while those with low self-compassion may be kinder to others than to themselves (Neff, 2003).  It is also possible that the “traditional” gender role of women as care-giver, helper, and self-sacrificing (Swickert et al., 2016) may be strongly influencing these participants.
 
Male identified participants
 
There was a great correlation between self-compassion and empathy for others among male identified participants, indicating that as self-compassion increases, empathy for others decreases.  One possible explanation for this finding could be the influence of gender roles.  Research suggests that men are expected to be individualistic and competitive (Willer et al., 2015), and to care more about social dominance (Stuijfzand et al., 2016). These factors do not necessarily facilitate empathic behaviors and indicate more of a focus on self than on others. Another explanation could be related to a study that involved a hypothetical situation of a person who made a mistake and needed assistance (Welp and Brown, 2013). Participants in this study who scored higher in self-compassion endorsed higher readiness to help, yet had low empathy and viewed the person as the cause of his problem. The authors considered empathy as sympathy for the other person, troubled by his situation, and anticipated distress if they were in the same situation. They noted that individuals who are not distressed by their own mistakes are unlikely to be distressed by mistakes of others, and provided two possible explanations for the lack of empathy: (1): self-compassionate participants did not appraise distress in the situation and/or (2) self-compassionate participants have less negative emotional reactivity and the situation did not meet the threshold.  Clearly, there are unique nuances in the correlation between self-compassion and empathy.
 
Implications for counseling centers
 
For those that work on university campuses and in college counseling centers, this information can  be  quite beneficial to both clinical practice and outreach programming.  Counselors in counseling centers may want to pay particular attention to gender identity when addressing self-compassion and empathy with their students both clinically and in outreach programming and workshops.  Counseling Centers may consider creating specific interpersonal therapy groups that address self-compassion and empathy based on gender identity, helping female identified students focus on building their self-compassion, while helping male identified students improve their empathy for others.  These groups can pointedly acknowledge and discuss gender roles and how these roles impact our beliefs about ourselves and our relationships with others. 
Outreach programming can target specific groups, like fraternities, sororities, and athletic teams, and educate them on self-compassion and empathy. Programming may also seek to educate students through their use of technology, as studies have shown that the presence of a mobile device negatively impacts empathic concern and connection with others (Misra et al., 2016).  Finally, outreach programs, such as dog therapy visits, mental health awareness days, and de-stress events after a semester is completed, can help to foster connections, help students to see that they are not alone, and show students that they share a common human experience with their peers. 


 LIMITATIONS

There are limitations to the generalizability of the findings in this work as the data were obtained from a single institution and solely from students taking a psychology course at that university.  Another limitation of the study is that those participants who identified with a non-binary gender identity were removed from data analyses due to a low sample size. It will be important for future research to include a more diverse sample and further examine the relationship of empathy for others and self-compassion in those who identify with a non-binary gender identity. Also the use of self-report procedures, which could be controlled by social desirability bias, reference bias, and response bias is a limitation of the study.

 


 CONFLICT OF INTERESTS

The authors have not declared any conflict of interests.

 



 REFERENCES

Arslan C (2016). Interpersonal problem solving, self-compassion and personality traits in university students. Educational Research and Reviews 11(7):474-481. 
Crossref

 

Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S (2004). The Empathy quotient: An investigation of adults with Asperger syndrome or high functioning

 
 

autism, and normal sex differences. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 34:163-175. 
Crossref

 
 

Birnie K, Speca M, Carlson LE (2010). Exploring self-compassion and empathy in the context of mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR). Stress and Health 26:359-371. 
Crossref

 
 

Brill M, Nahmani N (2017). The presence of compassion in therapy. Clinical Social Work Journal 45(1):10-21. 
Crossref

 
 

Carlo G, McGinley M, Hayes RC, Martinez MM (2012). Empathy as a mediator of the relations between parent and peer attachment and prosocial and physically aggressive behaviors in Mexican American college students. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 29:337-357. 
Crossref

 
 

Carre A, Stefaniak N, D'Ambrosio F, Bensalah L, Besche-Richard C (2013). The Basic Empathy Scale in Adults (BES-A): Factor structure of a revised form. Psychological Assessment 25(3):679-691. 
Crossref

 
 

Chang EC, Yu T, Najarian AS, Wright KM, Chen W, Chang OD, Du Y, Hirsch JK (2016). Understanding the association between negative life events and suicidal risk in colleg students: Examining self-compassion as a potential mediator. Journal of Clinical Psychology 0, 1-11. 
Crossref

 
 

Derntl B, Finkelmeyer A, Eickhoff SB, Kellermann T, Falkenberg I, Schneider F, Habel U (2010). Multidimensional assessment of empathic abilities: neural correlates and gender differences. Psychoneuroendocrinology 35(1):67-82. 
Crossref

 
 

Eisenberg N, Lennon R (1983). Sex differences in empathy and related capacities. Psychological Bulletin 94:100-131. 
Crossref

 
 

Jolliffe D, Farrington DP (2006). Development and validation of the Basic Empathy Scale. Journal of Adolescence 29:589-611. 
Crossref

 
 

Lam CB, Solmeyer AR, McHale SM (2012). Sibling relationships and empathy across the transition to adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence 41:1657-1670. 
Crossref

 
 

Leary MR, Tate EB, Adams CE, Allen AB, Hancock J (2007). Self-compassion and reactions to unpleasant self-relevant events: The implications of treating oneself kindly. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 92:887-904. 
Crossref

 
 

Lomax RG (2001). Statistical concepts: A second course for education and the behavioral sciences. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Publishers.

 
 

Misra S, Cheng L, Genevie J, Yuan M (2016). The iphone effect: The quality of in-person social interactions in the presence of mobile devices. Environment and Behavior 48(2):275-298. 
Crossref

 
 

Neff KD (2003). The development and validation of a scale to measure self- compassion. Self and Identity 2:223-250. 
Crossref

 
 

Neff KD (2011). Self-compassion: Stop beating yourself up and leave insecurity behind. New York, NY: HarperCollins Publishers.

 
 

Pavlova MA, Wecker M, Krombholz K, Sokolov AA (2010). Perception of intentions and actions: Gender stereotype susceptibility. Brain Research 1311:81-85. 
Crossref

 
 

Perry D, Walder K, Hendler T, Shamay-Tsoory SG (2013). The gender you are and the gender you like: Sexual preference and empathic neural responses. Brain Research 1534:66-75. 
Crossref

 
 

Rueckert L, Branch B, Doan T (2011). Are gender differences in empathy due to differences in emotional reactivity? Psychology 2(6):574-578. 
Crossref

 
 

Rueckert L, Naybar N (2008). Gender differences in empathy: The role of the right hemisphere. Brain and Cognition 67:162-167. 
Crossref

 
 

Schulte-Ruther M, Markowitsch HJ, Shah NJ, Fink GR, Piefke M (2008). Gender differences in brain networks supporting empathy.Neuroimag 42(1):393-403. 
Crossref

 
 

Stuijfzand S, De Wied M, Kempes M, Van de Graaff J, Branje S, Meeus W (2016). Gender differences in empathic sadness towards persons of the same- versus other-sex during adolescence. Sex Roles 75:434-446. 
Crossref

 
 

Swickert R, Robertson S, Baird D (2016). Age moderates the mediational role of empathy in the association between gender and forgiveness. Current Psychology: A Journal for Diverse Perspectives on Diverse Psychological Issues 35(3):354-360. 
Crossref

 
 

Taylor ZE, Doane LD, Eisenberg N (2014). Transitioning from high school to college: Relations of social support, ego-resiliency, and maladjustment during emerging adulthood. Emerging Adulthood 2(2):105-115.
Crossref

 
 

Taylor ZE, Eisenberg N, Spinrad TL, Eggum ND, Sulik M (2013). The relations of ego-resiliency and emotion socialization to the development of empathy and prosocial behavior across early childhood. Emotion 13(5):822-831. 
Crossref

 
 

Wei M, Yu-Hsin Liao K, Ku Y, Shaffer PA (2011). Attachment, self-compassion, empathy, and subjective well-being among college students and community adults. Journal of Personality 79(1):191-221. 
Crossref

 
 

Welp LR, Brown CM (2013). Self-compassion, empathy, and helping intentions. The Journal of Positive Psychology: Dedicated to furthering research and promoting good practice. 
Crossref

 
 

Willer R, Wimer C, Owens L (2015). What drives the gender gap in charitable giving? Lower empathy leads men to give less to poverty relief. Social Science 52:83-98. 
Crossref

 
 

Yang CY, Decety J, Lee S, Chen C, Cheng Y (2009). Gender differences in the mu rhythm during empathy for pain: An electroencephalographic study. Brain Research 1251:16-184. 
Crossref

 
 

Yarnell LM, Neff KD (2013). Self-compassion, interpersonal conflict resolutions, and well-being. Self and Identity 12(2):146-159. 
Crossref

 

 




          */?>